Linguistic anthropology: Difference between revisions

From Citizendium
Jump to navigation Jump to search
imported>Richard J. Senghas
(→‎current paradigm: adding links to hoped-for additions....)
mNo edit summary
 
(10 intermediate revisions by 5 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
'''Linguistic anthropology''' is that branch of [[anthropology]] that brings [[linguistics|linguistic methods]] to bear on anthropological problems, linking the analysis of semiotic and particularly linguistic forms and processes (on both small and large scales) to the interpretation of sociocultural processes (again on small and large scales).  In North American anthropology, linguistic anthropology is one of the traditional four primary fields, along with [[archaeology]], [[physical anthropology|physical]] or [[physical anthropology|biological anthropology]], and [[sociocultural anthropology]].
{{subpages}}
'''Linguistic anthropology''' is that branch of [[anthropology]] that brings [[linguistics|linguistic methods]] to bear on anthropological problems, linking the analysis of semiotic and particularly linguistic forms and processes (on both small and large scales) to the interpretation of sociocultural processes (again, on small and large scales).  In North American anthropology, linguistic anthropology is one of the traditional four primary fields, along with [[archaeology]], [[physical anthropology|physical]] or [[physical anthropology|biological anthropology]], and sociocultural anthropology.


===Three Paradigms===
===Three Paradigms===
====first paradigm====
====First paradigm====
As [[Duranti]] <ref name="Duranti 2003">Alessandro Duranti. 2003. Language as Culture in U.S. Anthropology: Three Paradigms. Current Anthropology 44(3):323-348.</ref> has noted (and the next paragraphs summarize his article), three paradigms have emerged over the history of the subdiscipline. The first paradigm, known as "'''linguistics'''" (later, "'''[[anthropological linguistics]]'''"), was devoted to themes unique to the subdiscipline—linguistic documentation of languages then seen as doomed to extinction (these were the languages of native North America on which the first members of the subdiscipline focused), grammatical description, [[typological classification]], and the problem/question of [[linguistic relativity]] (associated with [[Sapir, Edward|Sapir]] and [[Benjamin Whorf|Whorf]] but actually developed by [[Boas]] and, before him, by a long line of European thinkers from [[Vico]] to [[Herder]] to [[Humboldt]]).
As Duranti <ref name="Duranti 2003">Alessandro Duranti. 2003. Language as Culture in U.S. Anthropology: Three Paradigms. Current Anthropology 44(3):323-348.</ref> has noted (and the next paragraphs summarize his article), three paradigms have emerged over the history of the subdiscipline. The first paradigm, known as "linguistics" (later, "[[anthropological linguistics]]"), was devoted to themes unique to the subdiscipline—linguistic documentation of [[language (general)|languages]] then seen as doomed to [[language extinction|extinction]] (these were the languages of native North America on which the first members of the subdiscipline focused), [[descriptive linguistics|grammatical description]], [[Linguistic typology|typological]] classification, and the problem/question of [[linguistic relativity]] (associated with [[Edward Sapir]] and [[Benjamin Whorf]] but actually developed by [[Franz Boas]] and, before him, by a long line of European thinkers from [[Giambattista Vico|Vico]] to [[Johann Gottfried Herder|Herder]] to [[Wilhelm von Humboldt|Humboldt]]).


====second paradigm====
====Second paradigm====
[[Hymes, Dell|Dell Hymes]] was largely responsible for launching the second paradigm that fixed the name "linguistic anthropology" in the 1960s, though he also coined the term "'''[[ethnography of speaking]]'''" (or "'''ethnography of communication'''") to describe the agenda he envisioned for the field. It would involve taking advantage of new developments in technology, including new forms of mechanical recording. And Hymes introduced a new unit of analysis. Whereas the first paradigm focused on ostensibly distinct "languages" (scare quotes indicate that contemporary linguistic anthropologists treat the concept of "a language" as an ideal construction covering up complexities within and "across" so-called linguistic boundaries), the unit of analysis in the second paradigm was new—the "'''[[speech event]]'''." (The speech event is an event defined by the speech occurring in it— a lecture, for example—so that a dinner is not a speech event, but a speech situation, a situation in which speech may or may not occur.)
[[Dell Hymes]] was largely responsible for launching the second paradigm that fixed the name "linguistic anthropology" in the 1960s, though he also coined the term "[[ethnography of speaking]]" (or "ethnography of communication") to describe the agenda he envisioned for the field. It would involve taking advantage of new developments in [[technology]], including new forms of mechanical recording. Hymes also introduced a new unit of analysis. Whereas the first paradigm focused on ostensibly distinct "languages" (scare quotes indicate that contemporary linguistic anthropologists treat the concept of "a language" as an ideal construction covering up complexities within and "across" so-called linguistic boundaries), the unit of analysis in the second paradigm was new — the "[[speech event]]." (The speech event is an event defined by the speech occurring in it — a lecture, for example — so that a dinner is not a speech event, but a speech situation, a situation in which speech may or may not occur.)


Hymes had hoped to link linguistic anthropology more closely with the mother discipline. The name certainly stresses that the primary identity is with anthropology, whereas anthropological linguistics conveys a sense that the primary identity of *its* practitioners was with linguistics, which is a separate academic discipline on most universities campuses today (not in the days of Boas and Sapir). However, Hymes' ambition in a sense backfired; the second paradigm in fact marked a further distancing of the subdiscipline from the rest of anthropology.
Hymes had hoped to link linguistic anthropology more closely with the mother discipline. The name certainly stresses that the primary identity is with anthropology, whereas anthropological linguistics conveys a sense that the primary identity of *its* practitioners was with linguistics, which is a separate academic discipline on most universities campuses today (not in the days of Boas and Sapir). However, Hymes' ambition in a sense backfired; the second paradigm in fact marked a further distancing of the subdiscipline from the rest of anthropology.


====current paradigm====
====Current paradigm====
In the current paradigm, which has emerged since the late 1980s, instead of continuing to pursue agendas that come from a discipline alien to anthropology, linguistic anthropologists have systematically addressed themselves to problems posed by the larger discipline of anthropology—but using linguistic data and methods.
In the current paradigm, which has emerged since the late 1980s, instead of continuing to pursue agendas that come from a discipline alien to anthropology, linguistic anthropologists have systematically addressed themselves to problems posed by the larger discipline of anthropology — but using linguistic data and methods.


So, for example, they investigate questions of '''sociocultural identity''' *linguistically*. Linguistic anthropologist [[Don Kulick]] has done this in relation to identity, for example, in a series of settings, first in a village called Gapun in [[Papua New Guinea]]. <ref name="Kulick 1992">Don Kulick. 1992. Language Shift and Cultural Reproduction: Socialization, Self and Syncretism in a Papua New Guinea Village. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref> Kulick explored how the use of two languages with and around children in Gapun village—the traditional language ([[Taiap]]) not spoken anywhere but in their own village and thus primordially "indexical" of Gapuner identity, and [[Tok Pisin]] (the widely circulating official language of New Guinea). (Linguistic anthropologists use "indexical" to mean indicative, though some indexical signs create their indexical meanings on the fly, so to speak <ref name="Silverstein 1976">Michael Silverstein. 1976. Shifters, Linguistic Categories, and Cultural Description. In Meaning in Anthropology. K. Basso and H.A. Selby, eds. Pp. pp. 11-56. Albuquerque: School of American Research/ University of New Mexico Press.</ref>. To speak Taiap is associated with one identity—not only local but "Backward" and also an identity based on the display of *hed* (personal autonomy). To speak Tok Pisin is to index a Modern, Christian ([[Catholic]]) identity, based not on *hed* but on *save*, that is an identity linked with the will and the skill to cooperate. In later work <ref name="Kulick and Klein 2003">Don Kulick and Charles H. Klein. 2003. Scandalous Acts: The Politics of Shame among Brazilian Travesti Prostitutes. In Recognition Struggles and Social Movements: Contested Identities, Agency and Power. B. Hobson, ed. Pp. 215-238. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref>, Kulick demonstrates that certain loud speech performances called *um escândalo*, Brazilian travesti (roughly, 'transvestite') prostitutes shame clients. The travesti community, the argument goes, ends up at least making a powerful attempt to transcend the shame the larger Brazilian public might try to foist off on them—again, through loud public discourse and other modes of performance.
So, for example, they investigate questions of sociocultural identity ''linguistically''. Linguistic anthropologist [[Don Kulick]] has done this in relation to [[identity]], for example, in a series of settings, first in a village called Gapun in [[Papua New Guinea]].<ref name="Kulick 1992">Don Kulick. 1992. Language Shift and Cultural Reproduction: Socialization, Self and Syncretism in a Papua New Guinea Village. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref> Kulick explored how the use of two languages with and around children in Gapun village — the traditional language ([[Taiap language|Taiap]]) not spoken anywhere but in their own village and thus primordially "indexical" of Gapuner identity, and [[Tok Pisin]] (the widely circulating [[official language]] of New Guinea). (Linguistic anthropologists use "indexical" to mean indicative, though some indexical signs create their indexical meanings on the fly, so to speak <ref name="Silverstein 1976">Michael Silverstein. 1976. Shifters, Linguistic Categories, and Cultural Description. In Meaning in Anthropology. K. Basso and H.A. Selby, eds. Pp. pp. 11-56. Albuquerque: School of American Research/ University of New Mexico Press.</ref>. To speak Taiap is associated with one identity — not only local but "Backward" and also an identity based on the display of *hed* (personal autonomy). To speak Tok Pisin is to index a Modern, [[Christianity|Christian]] ([[Catholic]]) identity, based not on *hed* but on *save*, that is an identity linked with the will and the skill to cooperate. In later work <ref name="Kulick and Klein 2003">Don Kulick and Charles H. Klein. 2003. Scandalous Acts: The Politics of Shame among Brazilian Travesti Prostitutes. In Recognition Struggles and Social Movements: Contested Identities, Agency and Power. B. Hobson, ed. Pp. 215-238. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref>, Kulick demonstrates that certain loud speech performances called *um escândalo*, Brazilian travesti (roughly, 'transvestite') prostitutes shame clients. The travesti community, the argument goes, ends up at least making a powerful attempt to transcend the shame the larger Brazilian public might try to foist off on them—again, through loud public discourse and other modes of performance.
 
Linguistic anthropologists [[Elinor Ochs]] and [[Bambi Schieffelin]], in a remarkable series of studies <ref name="Ochs and Schieffelin 1984">Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin. 1984. Language Acquisition and Socialization: Three Developmental Stories and Their Implications. In Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion. R. Shweder and R.A. LeVine, eds. Pp. 276-320. New York: Cambridge University.</ref><ref name="Ochs 1988">Elinor Ochs. 1988. Culture and language development: Language acquisition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref><ref name="Ochs and Taylor 2001">Elinor Ochs and Carolyn Taylor. 2001. The “Father Knows Best” Dynamic in Dinnertime Narratives. In Linguistic Anthropology: A Reader. A. Duranti, ed. Pp. 431-449. Oxford. Malden, MA: Blackwell.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 1990">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 1990. The Give and Take of Everyday Life: Language Socialization of Kaluli Children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref> addressed the important anthropological topic of [[socialization]] (the process by which infants, children, and foreigners become members of a community, learning to participate in its culture), using linguistic as well as [[ethnography|ethnographic]] methods. They discovered that the processes of enculturation and socialization do not occur apart from the process of [[language acquisition]], but that children acquire language and culture together in what amounts to an integrated process. Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that [[baby talk]] is not universal, that the direction of adaptation (whether the child is made to adapt to the ongoing situation of speech around it or vice versa) was variable that correlated, for example, with the direction it was held vis-a-vis a caregiver's body. In many societies caregivers hold a child facing outward so as to orient it to a network of kin whom it must learn to recognize early in life.


Linguistic anthropologists [[Elinor Ochs]] and [[Bambi Schieffelin]], in a remarkable series of studies <ref name="Ochs and Schieffelin 1984">Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin. 1984. Language Acquisition and Socialization: Three Developmental Stories and Their Implications. In Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion. R. Shweder and R.A. LeVine, eds. Pp. 276-320. New York: Cambridge University.</ref><ref name="Ochs 1988">Elinor Ochs. 1988. Culture and language development: Language acquisition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref><ref name="Ochs and Taylor 2001">Elinor Ochs and Carolyn Taylor. 2001. The “Father Knows Best” Dynamic in Dinnertime Narratives. In Linguistic Anthropology: A Reader. A. Duranti, ed. Pp. 431-449. Oxford. Malden, MA: Blackwell.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 1990">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 1990. The Give and Take of Everyday Life: Language Socialization of Kaluli Children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref> addressed the important anthropological topic of '''[[socialization]]''' (the process by which infants, children, and foreigners become members of a community, learning to participate in its culture), using linguistic as well as [[ethnography|ethnographic]] methods. They discovered that the processes of enculturation and socialization do not occur apart from the process of language acquisition, but that children acquire language and culture together in what amounts to an integrated process. Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that [[baby talk]] is not universal, that the direction of adaptation (whether the child is made to adapt to the ongoing situation of speech around it or vice versa) was variable that correlated, for example, with the direction it was held vis-a-vis a caregiver's body. In many societies caregivers hold a child facing outward so as to orient it to a network of kin whom it must learn to recognize early in life.
Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that members of all societies socialize children both *to* and *through* the use of language. Ochs and Taylor uncovered how, through naturally occurring stories told during dinners in white middle class households in southern California, both mothers and fathers participated in replicating male dominance (the "father knows best" syndrome) by the distribution of participant roles such as protagonist (often a child but sometimes mother and almost never the father) and "problematizer" (often the father, who raised uncomfortable questions or challenged the competence of the protagonist). When mothers collaborated with children to get their stories told they unwittingly set themselves up to be subject to this process.
Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that members of all societies socialize children both *to* and *through* the use of language. Ochs and Taylor uncovered how, through naturally occurring stories told during dinners in white middle class households in southern California, both mothers and fathers participated in replicating male dominance (the "father knows best" syndrome) by the distribution of participant roles such as protagonist (often a child but sometimes mother and almost never the father) and "problematizer" (often the father, who raised uncomfortable questions or challenged the competence of the protagonist). When mothers collaborated with children to get their stories told they unwittingly set themselves up to be subject to this process.
Schieffelin's more recent research <ref name="Schieffelin 1995">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 1995. Creating evidence: Making sense of written words in Bosavi. Pragmatics 5(2):225-244.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 2000">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2000. Introducing Kaluli Literacy: A Chronology of Influences. In Regimes of Language. P. Kroskrity, ed. Pp. 293-327. Santa Fe: School of American Research Press.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 2002">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2002. Marking time: The dichotomizing discourse of multiple temporalities. Current Anthropology 43(Supplement):S5-17.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 2006">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2006. PLENARY ADDRESS: Found in translating: Reflexive language across time and texts in Bosavi, PNG. Twelve Annual Conference on Language, Interaction, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles, 2006.</ref> has uncovered the socializing role of pastors and other fairly new Bosavi converts in the Papua New Guinea community she studies. Pastors have introduced new ways of conveying knowledge— i.e. new linguistic epistemic markers<ref name="Schieffelin 1995" /> —and new ways of speaking about time <ref name="Schieffelin 2002">. And they have struggled with and largely resisted those parts of the Bible that speak of being able to know the inner states of others (e.g. the gospel of Mark, chapter 2, verses 6-8; <ref name="Schieffelin 2006" />.
In a third example of the current (third) paradigm, since Silverstein <ref name="Silverstein 1979">Michael Silverstein. 1979. Language Structure and Linguistic Ideology. In The Elements: A Parasession on Linguistic Units and Levels. R. Cline, W. Hanks, and C. Hofbauer, eds. Pp. pp. 193-247. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.</ref> opened the way, there has been an efflorescence of work done by linguistic anthropologists on the major anthropological theme of ideologies—in this case "'''[[linguistic ideologies]]'''," sometimes defined as "shared bodies of commonsense notions about the nature of language in the world" <ref name="Rumsey 1990">Alan Rumsey. 1990. Word, meaning, and linguistic ideology. American Anthropologist 92(2):346-361.</ref>. Silverstein <ref name="Silverstein 1985">Michael Silverstein. 1985. Language and the Culture of Gender: At the Intersection of Structure, Usage, and Ideology. In Semiotic Mediation: Sociocultural and Psychological Perspectives. E. Mertz and R. Parmentier, eds. Pp. 219-259. Orlando: Academic Press.</ref> has demonstrated that these ideologies are not mere false consciousness but actually influence the evolution of linguistic structures, including the dropping of "thee" and "thou" from everyday English usage. Woolard <ref name="Woolard 2004">Kathryn A. Woolard. 2004. Codeswitching. In Companion to Linguistic Anthropology. A. Duranti, ed. Pp. 73-94. Malden: Blackwell.</ref>, in her overview of "code switching," or the systematic practice of alternating linguistic varieties within a conversation or even a single utterance, finds the underlying question anthropologists ask of the practice—Why do they do that?—reflects a dominant linguistic ideology. It is the ideology that people should "really" be monoglot and effeciently targeted toward referential clarity rather than diverting themselves with the messiness of multiple codes in play at a single time.


In a final example of this third paradigm, a group of linguistic anthropologists has done very creative work on the idea of '''social space'''. Duranti published a ground breaking article on [[Samoan]] greetings and their use and transformation of social space <ref name="Duranti 1992">Alessandro Duranti. 1992. Language and Bodies in Social Space: Samoan Greetings. American Anthropologist 94:657-691.</ref>. Prior to that, Indonesianist Joseph Errington <ref name="Errington 1988">J. Joseph  Errington. 1988. Structure and Style in Javanese: A Semiotic View of Linguistic Etiquette. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.</ref>—making use of earlier work by Indonesianists not necessarily concerned with language issues per se—brought linguistic anthropological methods (and semiotic theory) to bear on the notion of the "exemplarly center," or the center of political and ritual power from which emanated exemplary behavior. Errington demonstrated how the Javanese *priyayi*, whose ancestors served at the Javanese royal courts, became emissaries, so to speak, long after those courts had ceased to exist, representing throughout Java the highest example of 'refined speech.' The work of Joel Kuipers further develops this theme vis-a-vis on the island of Sumba, Indonesia. And, even though it pertains to Tewa Indians in Arizona rather than Indonesians, Paul Kroskrity's <ref name="Kroskrity 1998">Paul V. Kroskrity. 1998. Arizona Tewa Kiva speech as a manifestation of linguistic ideology. In Language ideologies: Practice and theory. B.B. Schieffelin, K.A. Woolard, and P. Kroskrity, eds. Pp. 103-122. New York: Oxford University Press.</ref>argument that speech forms originating in the [[Tewa]] *[[kiva]]* (or underground ceremonial space) forms the dominant model for all Tewa speech can be seen as a rather direct parallel.
Schieffelin's more recent research <ref name="Schieffelin 1995">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 1995. Creating evidence: Making sense of written words in Bosavi. Pragmatics 5(2):225-244.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 2000">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2000. Introducing Kaluli Literacy: A Chronology of Influences. In Regimes of Language. P. Kroskrity, ed. Pp. 293-327. Santa Fe: School of American Research Press.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 2002">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2002. Marking time: The dichotomizing discourse of multiple temporalities. Current Anthropology 43(Supplement):S5-17.</ref><ref name="Schieffelin 2006">Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2006. PLENARY ADDRESS: Found in translating: Reflexive language across time and texts in Bosavi, PNG. Twelve Annual Conference on Language, Interaction, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles, 2006.</ref> has uncovered the socializing role of pastors and other fairly new Bosavi converts in the Papua New Guinea community she studies. Pastors have introduced new ways of conveying knowledge— i.e. new linguistic epistemic markers<ref name="Schieffelin 1995" /> —and new ways of speaking about time <ref name="Schieffelin 2002" />. And they have struggled with and largely resisted those parts of the Bible that speak of being able to know the inner states of others (e.g. the gospel of Mark, chapter 2, verses 6-8; <ref name="Schieffelin 2006" />.


Silverstein <ref name="Silverstein 2004">Michael Silverstein. 2004. "Cultural" Concepts and the Language-Culture Nexus. Current Anthropology 45(5):621-652.</ref> tries to find the maximum theoretical significance and applicability in this idea of exemplary centers. He feels, in fact, that the exemplary center idea is one of linguistic anthropology's three most important findings. He generalizes the notion in the following manner, arguing that “there are wider-scale institutional ‘orders of interactionality,’ historically contingent yet structured. Within such large-scale, macrosocial orders, in-effect ritual centers of semiosis come to exert a structuring, value-conferring influence on any particular event of discursive interaction with respect to the meanings and significance of the verbal and other semiotic forms used in it”<ref name="Silverstein 2004" /> (compare Wilce 2006). Current approaches to such classic anthropological topics as ritual by linguistic anthropologists emphasize not static linguistic structures but the unfolding in realtime of a "'hypertrophic' set of parallel orders of iconicity and indexicality that seem to cause the ritual to create its own sacred space through what appears, often, to be the magic of textual and nontextual metricalizations, synchronized" <ref name="Wilce 2006">James M. Wilce. 2006. Magical Laments and Anthropological Reflections: The Production and Circulation of Anthropological Text as Ritual Activity. Current Anthropology 47(6): 891-914.</ref><ref name="Silverstein 2004" />.
In a third example of the current (third) paradigm, since Silverstein <ref name="Silverstein 1979">Michael Silverstein. 1979. Language Structure and Linguistic Ideology. In The Elements: A Parasession on Linguistic Units and Levels. R. Cline, W. Hanks, and C. Hofbauer, eds. Pp. pp. 193-247. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.</ref> opened the way, there has been an efflorescence of work done by linguistic anthropologists on the major anthropological theme of ideologies—in this case "[[linguistic ideologies]]," sometimes defined as "shared bodies of commonsense notions about the nature of language in the world" <ref name="Rumsey 1990">Alan Rumsey. 1990. Word, meaning, and linguistic ideology. American Anthropologist 92(2):346-361.</ref>. Silverstein <ref name="Silverstein 1985">Michael Silverstein. 1985. Language and the Culture of Gender: At the Intersection of Structure, Usage, and Ideology. In Semiotic Mediation: Sociocultural and Psychological Perspectives. E. Mertz and R. Parmentier, eds. Pp. 219-259. Orlando: Academic Press.</ref> has demonstrated that these ideologies are not mere false consciousness but actually influence the evolution of linguistic structures, including the dropping of "thee" and "thou" from everyday English usage. Woolard <ref name="Woolard 2004">Kathryn A. Woolard. 2004. Codeswitching. In Companion to Linguistic Anthropology. A. Duranti, ed. Pp. 73-94. Malden: Blackwell.</ref>, in her overview of "code switching," or the systematic practice of alternating linguistic varieties within a conversation or even a single utterance, finds the underlying question anthropologists ask of the practice—Why do they do that?—reflects a dominant linguistic ideology. It is the ideology that people should "really" be monoglot and efficiently targeted toward referential clarity rather than diverting themselves with the messiness of multiple codes in play at a single time.


==References==
In a final example of this third paradigm, a group of linguistic anthropologists has done very creative work on the idea of social space. Duranti published a ground breaking article on [[Samoa]]n greetings and their use and transformation of social space <ref name="Duranti 1992">Alessandro Duranti. 1992. Language and Bodies in Social Space: Samoan Greetings. American Anthropologist 94:657-691.</ref>. Prior to that, [[Indonesia]]nist Joseph Errington <ref name="Errington 1988">J. Joseph  Errington. 1988. Structure and Style in Javanese: A Semiotic View of Linguistic Etiquette. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.</ref> — making use of earlier work by Indonesianists not necessarily concerned with language issues per se — brought linguistic anthropological methods (and semiotic theory) to bear on the notion of the "exemplarly center," or the center of political and ritual power from which emanated exemplary behavior. Errington demonstrated how the [[Java, Indonesia|Java]]nese *priyayi*, whose ancestors served at the Javanese royal courts, became emissaries, so to speak, long after those courts had ceased to exist, representing throughout Java the highest example of 'refined speech.' The work of Joel Kuipers further develops this theme vis-a-vis on the island of Sumba, Indonesia. And, even though it pertains to Tewa Indians in Arizona rather than Indonesians, Paul Kroskrity's <ref name="Kroskrity 1998">Paul V. Kroskrity. 1998. Arizona Tewa Kiva speech as a manifestation of linguistic ideology. In Language ideologies: Practice and theory. B.B. Schieffelin, K.A. Woolard, and P. Kroskrity, eds. Pp. 103-122. New York: Oxford University Press.</ref>argument that speech forms originating in the [[Tewa]] *[[kiva]]* (or underground ceremonial space) forms the dominant model for all Tewa speech can be seen as a rather direct parallel.
<div class="references-small" style="-moz-column-count:2; -webkit-column-count:2; column-count:2;">
<references />
</div>


== External links ==
Silverstein <ref name="Silverstein 2004">Michael Silverstein. 2004. "Cultural" Concepts and the Language-Culture Nexus. Current Anthropology 45(5):621-652.</ref> tries to find the maximum theoretical significance and applicability in this idea of exemplary centers. He feels, in fact, that the exemplary center idea is one of linguistic anthropology's three most important findings. He generalizes the notion, arguing that every instance of discourse is to some extent oriented toward such centers, or "institutional orders," which confer  value on (or withhold it from) such discursive events <ref name="Silverstein 2004" /> (compare Wilce 2006). Current linguistic anthropological approaches to such classic anthropological topics as ritual emphasize not static linguistic structures but the unfolding in realtime of complex "textual" patterns of signs, often involving layers of metaphors and other tropes. These emergent patternings appear, to participants, to create the very "sacred space" they address, or even appear to create the magicoreligious effects they aim at or invoke.<ref name="Wilce 2006">James M. Wilce. 2006. Magical Laments and Anthropological Reflections: The Production and Circulation of Anthropological Text as Ritual Activity. Current Anthropology 47(6): 891-914.</ref><ref name="Silverstein 2004" />
*[http://www.aaanet.org/sla/ Society for Linguistic Anthropology (SLA)], a section of the [http://www.aaanet.org American Anthropological Association].
*[http://www.aaanet.org/sla/jla/jlamain.htm Journal of Linguistic Anthropology], a quarterly journal of the SLA.
*[http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=LSY Language in Society], a quarterly journal.
*[http://www.indiana.edu/~anthling/ Anthropological Linguistics], a quarterly journal.


[[Category: Anthropology Workgroup]]
==Footnotes==
[[Category: Linguistics Workgroup]]
{{reflist|2}}[[Category:Suggestion Bot Tag]]
[[Category: CZ Live]]

Latest revision as of 07:01, 12 September 2024

This article is developed but not approved.
Main Article
Discussion
Related Articles  [?]
Bibliography  [?]
External Links  [?]
Citable Version  [?]
 
This editable, developed Main Article is subject to a disclaimer.

Linguistic anthropology is that branch of anthropology that brings linguistic methods to bear on anthropological problems, linking the analysis of semiotic and particularly linguistic forms and processes (on both small and large scales) to the interpretation of sociocultural processes (again, on small and large scales). In North American anthropology, linguistic anthropology is one of the traditional four primary fields, along with archaeology, physical or biological anthropology, and sociocultural anthropology.

Three Paradigms

First paradigm

As Duranti [1] has noted (and the next paragraphs summarize his article), three paradigms have emerged over the history of the subdiscipline. The first paradigm, known as "linguistics" (later, "anthropological linguistics"), was devoted to themes unique to the subdiscipline—linguistic documentation of languages then seen as doomed to extinction (these were the languages of native North America on which the first members of the subdiscipline focused), grammatical description, typological classification, and the problem/question of linguistic relativity (associated with Edward Sapir and Benjamin Whorf but actually developed by Franz Boas and, before him, by a long line of European thinkers from Vico to Herder to Humboldt).

Second paradigm

Dell Hymes was largely responsible for launching the second paradigm that fixed the name "linguistic anthropology" in the 1960s, though he also coined the term "ethnography of speaking" (or "ethnography of communication") to describe the agenda he envisioned for the field. It would involve taking advantage of new developments in technology, including new forms of mechanical recording. Hymes also introduced a new unit of analysis. Whereas the first paradigm focused on ostensibly distinct "languages" (scare quotes indicate that contemporary linguistic anthropologists treat the concept of "a language" as an ideal construction covering up complexities within and "across" so-called linguistic boundaries), the unit of analysis in the second paradigm was new — the "speech event." (The speech event is an event defined by the speech occurring in it — a lecture, for example — so that a dinner is not a speech event, but a speech situation, a situation in which speech may or may not occur.)

Hymes had hoped to link linguistic anthropology more closely with the mother discipline. The name certainly stresses that the primary identity is with anthropology, whereas anthropological linguistics conveys a sense that the primary identity of *its* practitioners was with linguistics, which is a separate academic discipline on most universities campuses today (not in the days of Boas and Sapir). However, Hymes' ambition in a sense backfired; the second paradigm in fact marked a further distancing of the subdiscipline from the rest of anthropology.

Current paradigm

In the current paradigm, which has emerged since the late 1980s, instead of continuing to pursue agendas that come from a discipline alien to anthropology, linguistic anthropologists have systematically addressed themselves to problems posed by the larger discipline of anthropology — but using linguistic data and methods.

So, for example, they investigate questions of sociocultural identity linguistically. Linguistic anthropologist Don Kulick has done this in relation to identity, for example, in a series of settings, first in a village called Gapun in Papua New Guinea.[2] Kulick explored how the use of two languages with and around children in Gapun village — the traditional language (Taiap) not spoken anywhere but in their own village and thus primordially "indexical" of Gapuner identity, and Tok Pisin (the widely circulating official language of New Guinea). (Linguistic anthropologists use "indexical" to mean indicative, though some indexical signs create their indexical meanings on the fly, so to speak [3]. To speak Taiap is associated with one identity — not only local but "Backward" and also an identity based on the display of *hed* (personal autonomy). To speak Tok Pisin is to index a Modern, Christian (Catholic) identity, based not on *hed* but on *save*, that is an identity linked with the will and the skill to cooperate. In later work [4], Kulick demonstrates that certain loud speech performances called *um escândalo*, Brazilian travesti (roughly, 'transvestite') prostitutes shame clients. The travesti community, the argument goes, ends up at least making a powerful attempt to transcend the shame the larger Brazilian public might try to foist off on them—again, through loud public discourse and other modes of performance.

Linguistic anthropologists Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin, in a remarkable series of studies [5][6][7][8] addressed the important anthropological topic of socialization (the process by which infants, children, and foreigners become members of a community, learning to participate in its culture), using linguistic as well as ethnographic methods. They discovered that the processes of enculturation and socialization do not occur apart from the process of language acquisition, but that children acquire language and culture together in what amounts to an integrated process. Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that baby talk is not universal, that the direction of adaptation (whether the child is made to adapt to the ongoing situation of speech around it or vice versa) was variable that correlated, for example, with the direction it was held vis-a-vis a caregiver's body. In many societies caregivers hold a child facing outward so as to orient it to a network of kin whom it must learn to recognize early in life.

Ochs and Schieffelin demonstrated that members of all societies socialize children both *to* and *through* the use of language. Ochs and Taylor uncovered how, through naturally occurring stories told during dinners in white middle class households in southern California, both mothers and fathers participated in replicating male dominance (the "father knows best" syndrome) by the distribution of participant roles such as protagonist (often a child but sometimes mother and almost never the father) and "problematizer" (often the father, who raised uncomfortable questions or challenged the competence of the protagonist). When mothers collaborated with children to get their stories told they unwittingly set themselves up to be subject to this process.

Schieffelin's more recent research [9][10][11][12] has uncovered the socializing role of pastors and other fairly new Bosavi converts in the Papua New Guinea community she studies. Pastors have introduced new ways of conveying knowledge— i.e. new linguistic epistemic markers[9] —and new ways of speaking about time [11]. And they have struggled with and largely resisted those parts of the Bible that speak of being able to know the inner states of others (e.g. the gospel of Mark, chapter 2, verses 6-8; [12].

In a third example of the current (third) paradigm, since Silverstein [13] opened the way, there has been an efflorescence of work done by linguistic anthropologists on the major anthropological theme of ideologies—in this case "linguistic ideologies," sometimes defined as "shared bodies of commonsense notions about the nature of language in the world" [14]. Silverstein [15] has demonstrated that these ideologies are not mere false consciousness but actually influence the evolution of linguistic structures, including the dropping of "thee" and "thou" from everyday English usage. Woolard [16], in her overview of "code switching," or the systematic practice of alternating linguistic varieties within a conversation or even a single utterance, finds the underlying question anthropologists ask of the practice—Why do they do that?—reflects a dominant linguistic ideology. It is the ideology that people should "really" be monoglot and efficiently targeted toward referential clarity rather than diverting themselves with the messiness of multiple codes in play at a single time.

In a final example of this third paradigm, a group of linguistic anthropologists has done very creative work on the idea of social space. Duranti published a ground breaking article on Samoan greetings and their use and transformation of social space [17]. Prior to that, Indonesianist Joseph Errington [18] — making use of earlier work by Indonesianists not necessarily concerned with language issues per se — brought linguistic anthropological methods (and semiotic theory) to bear on the notion of the "exemplarly center," or the center of political and ritual power from which emanated exemplary behavior. Errington demonstrated how the Javanese *priyayi*, whose ancestors served at the Javanese royal courts, became emissaries, so to speak, long after those courts had ceased to exist, representing throughout Java the highest example of 'refined speech.' The work of Joel Kuipers further develops this theme vis-a-vis on the island of Sumba, Indonesia. And, even though it pertains to Tewa Indians in Arizona rather than Indonesians, Paul Kroskrity's [19]argument that speech forms originating in the Tewa *kiva* (or underground ceremonial space) forms the dominant model for all Tewa speech can be seen as a rather direct parallel.

Silverstein [20] tries to find the maximum theoretical significance and applicability in this idea of exemplary centers. He feels, in fact, that the exemplary center idea is one of linguistic anthropology's three most important findings. He generalizes the notion, arguing that every instance of discourse is to some extent oriented toward such centers, or "institutional orders," which confer value on (or withhold it from) such discursive events [20] (compare Wilce 2006). Current linguistic anthropological approaches to such classic anthropological topics as ritual emphasize not static linguistic structures but the unfolding in realtime of complex "textual" patterns of signs, often involving layers of metaphors and other tropes. These emergent patternings appear, to participants, to create the very "sacred space" they address, or even appear to create the magicoreligious effects they aim at or invoke.[21][20]

Footnotes

  1. Alessandro Duranti. 2003. Language as Culture in U.S. Anthropology: Three Paradigms. Current Anthropology 44(3):323-348.
  2. Don Kulick. 1992. Language Shift and Cultural Reproduction: Socialization, Self and Syncretism in a Papua New Guinea Village. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  3. Michael Silverstein. 1976. Shifters, Linguistic Categories, and Cultural Description. In Meaning in Anthropology. K. Basso and H.A. Selby, eds. Pp. pp. 11-56. Albuquerque: School of American Research/ University of New Mexico Press.
  4. Don Kulick and Charles H. Klein. 2003. Scandalous Acts: The Politics of Shame among Brazilian Travesti Prostitutes. In Recognition Struggles and Social Movements: Contested Identities, Agency and Power. B. Hobson, ed. Pp. 215-238. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  5. Elinor Ochs and Bambi Schieffelin. 1984. Language Acquisition and Socialization: Three Developmental Stories and Their Implications. In Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion. R. Shweder and R.A. LeVine, eds. Pp. 276-320. New York: Cambridge University.
  6. Elinor Ochs. 1988. Culture and language development: Language acquisition and language socialization in a Samoan village. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  7. Elinor Ochs and Carolyn Taylor. 2001. The “Father Knows Best” Dynamic in Dinnertime Narratives. In Linguistic Anthropology: A Reader. A. Duranti, ed. Pp. 431-449. Oxford. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  8. Bambi B. Schieffelin. 1990. The Give and Take of Everyday Life: Language Socialization of Kaluli Children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  9. 9.0 9.1 Bambi B. Schieffelin. 1995. Creating evidence: Making sense of written words in Bosavi. Pragmatics 5(2):225-244.
  10. Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2000. Introducing Kaluli Literacy: A Chronology of Influences. In Regimes of Language. P. Kroskrity, ed. Pp. 293-327. Santa Fe: School of American Research Press.
  11. 11.0 11.1 Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2002. Marking time: The dichotomizing discourse of multiple temporalities. Current Anthropology 43(Supplement):S5-17.
  12. 12.0 12.1 Bambi B. Schieffelin. 2006. PLENARY ADDRESS: Found in translating: Reflexive language across time and texts in Bosavi, PNG. Twelve Annual Conference on Language, Interaction, and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles, 2006.
  13. Michael Silverstein. 1979. Language Structure and Linguistic Ideology. In The Elements: A Parasession on Linguistic Units and Levels. R. Cline, W. Hanks, and C. Hofbauer, eds. Pp. pp. 193-247. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.
  14. Alan Rumsey. 1990. Word, meaning, and linguistic ideology. American Anthropologist 92(2):346-361.
  15. Michael Silverstein. 1985. Language and the Culture of Gender: At the Intersection of Structure, Usage, and Ideology. In Semiotic Mediation: Sociocultural and Psychological Perspectives. E. Mertz and R. Parmentier, eds. Pp. 219-259. Orlando: Academic Press.
  16. Kathryn A. Woolard. 2004. Codeswitching. In Companion to Linguistic Anthropology. A. Duranti, ed. Pp. 73-94. Malden: Blackwell.
  17. Alessandro Duranti. 1992. Language and Bodies in Social Space: Samoan Greetings. American Anthropologist 94:657-691.
  18. J. Joseph Errington. 1988. Structure and Style in Javanese: A Semiotic View of Linguistic Etiquette. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania.
  19. Paul V. Kroskrity. 1998. Arizona Tewa Kiva speech as a manifestation of linguistic ideology. In Language ideologies: Practice and theory. B.B. Schieffelin, K.A. Woolard, and P. Kroskrity, eds. Pp. 103-122. New York: Oxford University Press.
  20. 20.0 20.1 20.2 Michael Silverstein. 2004. "Cultural" Concepts and the Language-Culture Nexus. Current Anthropology 45(5):621-652.
  21. James M. Wilce. 2006. Magical Laments and Anthropological Reflections: The Production and Circulation of Anthropological Text as Ritual Activity. Current Anthropology 47(6): 891-914.